Chapter 11 Lab 11: Mixed Factorial ANOVA

No amount of experimentation can ever prove me right; a single experiment can prove me wrong —Albert Einstein

11.1 Do you remember things better when you take pictures of them?

People take pictures of things all the time on their phones. Barasch et al. (2017) asked whether taking pictures of things had consequences for later memory of those experiences.

11.1.1 Study description

In Experiment 1, participants visited a museum exhibit. Half of the participants were allowed to take photographs (with camera, at least 10 pictures) and the other half were not (no camera). They freely looked at anything in the exhibit, and were allowed to take pictures of anything they wanted (if they were in the camera condition). Additionally, while visiting the exhibit, participants listened to audio guides about the things the were looking at.

After participants were done with the exhibit the returned to the sign-in desk. At this point they were given two memory tests for the things they saw and heard in the exhibit. They were given a visual recognition test containing pictures of objects, and were asked to identify which objects they remembered seeing. They were also given an auditory recognition test containing statements that could have been on the audio guide, and they had to identify which ones they had heard before.

This is a 2x2 mixed design. IV 1 was a between-subjects manipulation involving picture-taking (camera vs. no camera). IV2 was a within-subject manipulation of memory test (visual vs. audio). The dependent measure was performance on the memory test.

An overarching question was whether or not participants would have better visual memory for exhibit objects when they took pictures, compared to when they didn’t. Additionally, taking pictures or not, may have no influence on memory for the statements in the audio guide.

11.2 Lab Skills Learned

  • Conducting a 2x2 mixed design ANOVA

11.3 Important Stuff

  • citation: Barasch, A., Diehl, K., Silverman, J., & Zauberman, G. (2017). Photographic memory: The effects of volitional photo taking on memory for visual and auditory aspects of an experience. Psychological science, 28(8), 1056-1066.
  • Link to .pdf of article
  • Data in .csv format

11.4 R

11.4.1 Load the data

Remember that any line with a # makes a comment and the code does not run. Below is how to load the .csv data from the online repository, or from a local file (you need to change the file path to where the local file is, if you downloaded it). The data contains all of the measures and conditions from Experiment 1 in the paper.

library(data.table)
#all_data <- fread("https://github.com/CrumpLab/statisticsLab/raw/master/stroop_stand.csv")
all_data <- fread("data/study1_data_photo.csv")

11.4.2 Inspect the dataframe

This will give you a big picture of the data frame. Click the button to view it in your browser, then take a look to see what is in it.

library(summarytools)
view(dfSummary(all_data))

11.4.3 Get the data you need

This data file contains all of the data from Experiment 1 in the paper.

11.4.4 Get the data into the format you want

The data is sort of in long-format, we will see that we need to do some transformation after looking more closely at the independent and dependent variables

11.4.4.1 The independent variables

  1. Photo vs. No photo is coded in the condition column
  2. Memory task (visual vs. audio). There are no column variables describing which memory task people performed. Instead, the percent correct for the visual memory task is coded in the vpercent column, and the percent correct for the audio memory task is coded in the apercent column. We will need to transform the data, we do that after describing the DV

11.4.4.2 The dependent variable

In a sense there are two dependent variables, vpercent and apercent. However, we will treat them as a single memory performance variable.

11.4.4.3 Transforming the data

There were 297 participants. You can check that we have data for each participant by looking at the labID column.

length(unique(all_data$labID))
## [1] 297

There are 297 rows of data, each corresponding to a single subject. Let’s build a data frame we can use for analysis

subjects <- as.factor(c(all_data$labID,all_data$labID))
memory_accuracy <- c(all_data$vpercent,all_data$apercent)
memory_task<-as.factor(rep(c("visual","audio"), each=297))
camera <- as.factor(c(all_data$condition,all_data$condition))

new_data <- data.frame(subjects,
                       memory_accuracy,
                       memory_task,
                       camera)

11.4.5 Look at the data

Remember before we do any analysis, we always want to “look” at the data. This first pass let’s us know if the data “look right”. For example, the data file could be messed up and maybe there aren’t any numbers there, or maybe the numbers are just too weird. For example, the memory performance DV is a percent, so it should range between 0 and 100, if we find numbers that are negative or greater than 100, then we know something is wrong.

hist(new_data$memory_accuracy)

range(new_data$memory_accuracy)
## [1]  11.11111 100.00000

Great, the numbers check out, they are all inside the range of 0 to 100.

11.4.6 Look at the means

As part of looking at the data, let’s graph the means in each condition.

  1. Get the means for each condition, by averaging over the means for each subject. These are put into the data frame called plot_means.

  2. Make a graph with the plot_means data frame using ggplot2.

library(dplyr)
library(ggplot2)

plot_means <- new_data %>%
            group_by(memory_task,camera) %>%
            summarise(mean_accuracy = mean(memory_accuracy),
                      SEM = sd(memory_accuracy)/sqrt(length(memory_accuracy)))

ggplot(plot_means, aes(x=memory_task, y=mean_accuracy, group=camera, fill=camera))+
  geom_bar(stat="identity", position="dodge")+
  geom_errorbar(aes(ymin=mean_accuracy-SEM, ymax=mean_accuracy+SEM), 
                position=position_dodge(width=0.9),
                width=.2)+
  theme_classic()+
  coord_cartesian(ylim=c(50,100))

11.4.7 Conduct the ANOVA

This is a 2x2 mixed design. So, we need to run a mixed design ANOVA. The formula for running a mixed design ANOVA is very similar what we have seen before. The only difference is that we specify which IV is within-subjects by placing it in the error term:

aov(DV ~ IVB * IVW + Error(Subjects/IVW, data)

library(xtable)

aov_out<-aov(memory_accuracy ~ camera*memory_task + Error(subjects/memory_task), new_data)
summary_out<-summary(aov_out)

knitr::kable(xtable(summary_out))
Df Sum Sq Mean Sq F value Pr(>F)
camera 1 641.9545 641.9545 2.26222 0.1336337
Residuals 295 83712.7032 283.7719 NA NA
memory_task 1 26786.5902 26786.5902 108.84808 0.0000000
camera:memory_task 1 3394.1862 3394.1862 13.79237 0.0002441
Residuals 295 72597.0014 246.0915 NA NA

Let’s also print out the means for the main effects and interaction:

print(model.tables(aov_out,"means"), format="markdown")
## Tables of means
## Grand mean
##          
## 69.86532 
## 
##  camera 
##     nophoto  photo
##       68.83  70.91
## rep  298.00 296.00
## 
##  memory_task 
##      audio visual
##      76.58  63.15
## rep 297.00 297.00
## 
##  camera:memory_task 
##          memory_task
## camera    audio  visual
##   nophoto  77.93  59.73
##   rep     149.00 149.00
##   photo    75.23  66.59
##   rep     148.00 148.00

11.4.8 Generalization Exercise

The generalization exercise is the writing assignment for this lab.

11.4.9 Writing assignment

Your writing assignment is to write up the results of the ANOVA that you just conducted. You can follow the general recipe from the ANOVA write-up from the previous lab on factorial designs. Your write up will be in two parts

(3 points total)

Part 1

  1. Say what the numbers were that you are analyzing
  2. Say what the statistical test was
  3. Say the results of the statistical test for each main effect and interaction
  4. For each effect, say what the patterns of the means were.

Part 2

Conduct follow-up t-tests.

A. Determine whether memory accuracy is better in the camera vs no camera condition for the visual memory task. Report the t-test and the pattern of means.

B. Determine whether memory accuracy is better in the camera vs. no camera condition for the audio memory task. Report the t-test and the pattern of means.

11.5 Excel

How to do it in Excel

11.6 SPSS

In this lab, we will use SPSS to:

  1. Conduct and graph a Mixed-Factorial Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)
  2. Calculate simple effects

11.6.1 Experiment Background


People take pictures of things all the time on their phones. Barasch et al. (2017) asked whether taking pictures of things had consequences for later memory of those experiences.

In this experiment participants visited a museum exhibit. Half the participants were able to use a camera to document their visit, while the other half were not, all while listening to an audio tour of the exhibit. At the end of the visit, everyone was given two memory tests (visual and audio) for the content they saw and heard, respectively.

This is a 2x2 Mixed-Factorial design. The individuals in the photo group are different than the individuals in the no photo group (this is our between-subjects variable–it is called condition), while the memory test_type (audio and visual) is our within-subjects variable since everyone took both types of tests.

11.6.2 Conduct a Mixed-Factorial Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)


Using SPSS, open the data file linked here. It is called “study1_data_photo.sav”. It should look like this:

Scroll through the file. Find the variable named condition. This variable tells you if the row in question corresponds to a person who was or was not able to use a camera. Now, find the variables apercent and vpercent. These variables refer to the percentage score received by everyone on the audio and visual memory tests, respectively.

Notice that every row in the data file only has ONE “condition” label. That is, you were either in the camera or no camera condition. BUT, everyone in the file has scores for BOTH tests, audio and visual. This is the way your data must be structed in SPSS in order to perform a mixed-factorial ANOVA. Now, let’s begin.

Go to the top menu and choose Analyze, General Linear Model , and Repeated Measures…

The next window will ask for you to specify the Within-Subjects variable ONLY. Our within-subjects variable (the variable for which participants were measured across all levels) was the memory test_type. Enter test_type into this field, and specify that there are 2 levels. You must click Add in order to continue:

Then click Define. The next window will ask you to specify the levels of your within-subjects variable. Place apercent and vpercent from the list on the left into the field labeled “Within-Subjects Variables”. Then, place condition into the field labeled “Between-Subjects variables”.

Now, click Plots. Place condition in the “Separate Lines” field and test_type in the “Horizontal axis” field:

Then click Add. Your window will look like this once you’ve done that:

Now, click Continue and then OK. SPSS will produce output tables for your ANOVA, as well as a plot:

Let’s look at our output tables. Based on its values, we can tell that:

  1. There is a significant main effect of test type, F(1, 295)=108.59, p<.05. Overall, memory test scores were higher for the audio test than the video test.

  2. There is a significant ineraction effect between test type and condition, F(1, 295)=13.79, p<.05. People taking photos did worse on the audio test than people without photos, however, on the visual test, people taking photos did better than those who did not take photos.

  3. There is no significant main effect of condition, F(1, 295)=2.26, p=NS.

11.6.3 Calculate simple effects


What if we asked the questions: 1. In the no photo condition only, was there a difference between audio and visual memory score? 2. In the photo condition only, was there a difference between audio and visual memory score? 3. Were audio memory test scores different between photo and no photo conditions? 4. Were visual memory test scores different between photo and no photo conditions?

To answer these questions, we must run simple effects. If you remember from earlier labs, we must use SPSS syntax to do this. Go to Analyze, then General Linear Model, then Repeated Measures…

In the next window, you’ll find your settings from the previously-run ANOVA have been saved.

Click Define. The following window will appear:

Click Paste and the SPSS Syntax Editor will appear:

Change the code on the screen to the following:

GLM apercent vpercent BY condition
/WSFACTOR=test_type 2 Polynomial
/METHOD=SSTYPE(3)
/EMMEANS=TABLES(test_type * condition) compare(test_type) adj(lsd)
/EMMEANS=TABLES(test_type * condition) compare(condition) adj(lsd)
/CRITERIA=ALPHA(.05)
/WSDESIGN=test_type
/DESIGN=condition.

Click the green triangle (“play”) at the top of the window. The following two tables will appear (among others) in your output window:

Keep in mind that for test_type, 1=audio and 2=visual . According to the table, we can see that:

  1. In the no photo condition only, there was a significant difference between audio and visual memory (p<.05)?
  2. In the photo condition only, there was a significant difference between audio and visual memory (p<.05)?
  3. Audio memory test scores in the photo condition were not significantly different from those in the no photo condition (p=NS).
  4. Visual memory test scores in the photo condition were significantly different from those in the no photo condition (p<.05).

11.6.4 Practice Problems


  1. Using the same data file we used for this lab’s tutorial, run a Mixed-Factorial ANOVA using test type (audio and visual) and Gender (Male, Female). Use an alpha level of .05. Report your results in standard statistical reporting format.

  2. Produce a plot of means.

11.7 JAMOVI

How to do it in JAMOVI